Research Literacy: Understanding Results of Clinical Trials

diagram of the scientific method as an ongoing process

We’re regularly bombarded with news of the latest scientific research findings, and sometimes it seems like you can find a study to tell you just about anything. My concern with news reporting of research findings is that many people (including members of the media) have relatively limited research literacy. Research literacy refers to the ability to draw upon a body of knowledge about research methods in order to interpret and critically evaluate research findings. Greater research literacy means, among other things, a more effective bullshit detector.

In this post, I’ll break down some of the terminology that’s often used in research. The concepts involved are not necessarily intuitive, so building research literacy involves learning some fundamentals. The randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled trial is considered the “gold standard” for much medical research, and it’s useful to understand why that is.

Peer review

Peer review is a process by which fellow researchers who are subject matter experts give feedback on research papers before they get published. This is an important quality control step. Reviewers are independent of the journal that the paper has been submitted to. The journal’s editors select them because of their expertise in the field, and they give feedback to guide the editor in making a decision about whether or not to publish a manuscript. Most often, reviewers will recommend improvements be made before a paper is accepted.

The peer review process can potentially get a little shakier when it comes to “open access” journals. The articles in these journals are publicly available without a subscription. Instead, the journal’s revenue stream comes from authors, who must pay a publication fee. Some of these journals are credible and reputable, while others are predatory.  

Because I’ve had work published in academic journals, my email address is out there in that world. That means that I regularly get emails from obscure open access journals asking me to do reviews for papers that are so completely unrelated to my research field it’s sometimes laughable. Hmmmm.

Quantitative vs qualitative research

Quantitative research collects quantifiable data and generally uses statistical methods to analyze that data. Experiments are one type of quantitative research design. Quantitative research is the basis of much of the medical knowledge that we have. However, numbers can never tell the whole story, and that’s where qualitative research comes in.

Qualitative research uses people’s stories to answer questions like what does it feel like to have mental illness, for example. I’m partial to a method called autoethnography, which is a study of the self in the context of culture. I used this in my master’s thesis to examine my own experiences of mental illness, and it’s a really exciting way for the voices of people with mental illness to be heard.

Qualitative research is often considered as producing lower-strength evidence than quantitative research. However, it can be an excellent way to provide richer detail of what is studied quantitatively. For example, a quantitative study might look at the effectiveness of interventions to reduce the use of seclusion and restraints in a psychiatric ICU, while a qualitative study might involve talking to the patients in the ICU and getting their descriptions of what it felt like to experience these different types of interventions.

Experimental design

Experimental design involves researchers manipulating variables and evaluating the outcomes. This type of design can support cause-and-effect inferences. The gold standard for clinical trials is a randomized, double-blinded, placebo-controlled experimental design. Let’s break down what that means.

Randomization

There is a great deal of person-to-person variability in health status, genetics, and many other factors. By randomizing which participants are assigned to the intervention group and which are assigned to the control group, the likelihood decreases that one particular group will be stacked with people having certain characteristics. Greater homogeneity between participants receiving intervention A and those receiving intervention B improves the validity of the results obtained.

Blinding

Blinding refers to who knows what intervention the patient is receiving. In a double-blinded study, neither the patient nor the investigators evaluating the outcomes know this. Blinding is useful because it reduces the chance of bias in the results; humans are naturally prone to cognitive biases, and research methods try to compensate for that.

In a study of drug A, drug A and a placebo might be contained in identical gelatin capsules. Each participant would receive a numerically coded dose, and a member of the research team not involved in administering the drug or evaluating the participant’s response would be responsible for managing the codes. Specifics vary from study to study, but the purpose is still the same.

Placebo and other controls

Let’s say you give drug A to 20 people. A month later, 15 of them are doing better. That’s great, but it doesn’t tell you the extent to which drug A is or is not affecting outcomes. That’s where controls come in. A control represents a comparison group that allows researchers to make some determination of what is causing what. Using a placebo control is often the most desirable to separate out how much of the response is actually due to the drug being studied. The placebo arm would account for both people who had a positive response to the placebo and people who would have gotten better anyway with no intervention.

Sometimes, researchers will use another active drug as a control. One example of this is in studies of ketamine for major depressive disorder. Since ketamine is a dissociative anaesthetic, giving a normal saline infusion as a placebo would throw any attempts at blinding right out the window. Therefore, studies might use something like intravenous midazolam, a rapid- and short-acting benzodiazepine, as a control.

Other types of controls include “treatment as usual” (receiving whatever treatment is considered standard practice) or “waitlist” (patients who are currently on a waitlist to receive the intervention that’s being studied).

lab vials
PublicDomainPictures on Pixabay

Relative vs. absolute risk

Risk is often quantified as a percentage, but a number needs sufficient context to be meaningful.

Absolute risk refers to the probability of something happening, full stop.

Relative risk refers to the probability of something happening in relation to some other designated population, factor, or situation. Relative risk numbers are meaningless unless you know the reference point. Let’s say drug A causes a 500% increase in the risk of cancer X. Sounds scary, right? But what if the absolute risk of cancer X is 0.00001%? That means taking drug A increases the risk of cancer X to 0.00005%, which is a lot less frightening.

Reports on the news may talk about an increased risk of death from drug B. Again, that sounds pretty scary. Except the absolute risk of death is 100%, with no exceptions (at least thus far in human evolution). That means we need more information. What time frame are we talking about? Risk of death within 1 year? Within 20 years? What age group were the people involved? It makes a difference, and greater research literacy helps us to understand that.

Correlation vs causation

Correlation refers to things that tend to happen together. Sometimes, people will mistakenly assume that correlation indicates a causative relationship (and the illusion of causality is a psychological effect that leads us to see causal relationships in unrelated things). If we looked at 100 people who had died, we would find that 100% of them had skin. Does that mean having skin causes death? Of course not.  

This is a common type of mistake in the anti-vaxxer movement. Autism can show up at around the same age as children receive some vaccines, but that doesn’t mean there is a causative relationship. Causation can be difficult to establish definitively, which is why it’s important to have well-designed, rigorous, peer-reviewed research studies.

Statistical significance

The term “significant” means something different in research studies than in general usage. Significant results in a study indicate that, from a statistical perspective, the results obtained were unlikely to be due to chance. It does not mean that the results are large in scale (this would be described as a large effect size), nor does it mean that the results are necessarily important or meaningful (clinically significant).

Related to this is the concept of confidence intervals. These are a range that results would be expected to fall within a certain percentage of the time, such as 95%.

As a completely arbitrary example, say placebo was associated with a 10% improvement in symptom X, with a 95% confidence interval of +/- 5% (i.e. 5-15%). Study drug A was associated with 25% improvement in symptom X with a 95% confidence interval of +/- 5% (i.e. 20-30%). Because drug A is associated with a greater improvement in symptom X, and the lower end of its confidence interval doesn’t overlap with the upper end of the placebo confidence interval, the results are considered statistically significant. These numbers are completely made up, but hopefully, this example gives you the right idea.

Systematic reviews

Review papers evaluate the existing research literature on a topic. “Systematic” refers to clearly laid out criteria for inclusion and exclusion of studies in the review and elements used to evaluate the quality of the included studies. Review articles are a great way to get a lot of information all in one place that’s already been carefully scrutinized.

Conclusion

So there it is, a quick overview of bringing a critical eye to research. The best way to improve research literacy is by actually consuming research literature. This is where Google Scholar can be your new best friend. Full research papers often aren’t available for free access, but for the most part, there is public access to a paper’s abstract, which is a concise summary of the key points including method, results, and conclusions.

PubMed is another great source of information, and often hits on Google Scholar link to the corresponding PubMed page. Papers with the results of research funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) in the United States are available for free on PubMed. I’ve read a lot of research papers over the years, and while they can be a bit dry, they can also be an invaluable source of information. And the more you read the easier it gets. Trust me. And improving your research literacy is a great way to make sure your bullshit detector is as well-tuned as possible.

The science corner: pseudoscience, public health, and media literacy

Writing about science and debunking pseudoscience makes my heart sing! Visit the How to Spot Pseudoscience to explore other Science Corner posts on Mental Health @ Home.

23 thoughts on “Research Literacy: Understanding Results of Clinical Trials”

    1. I’m writing a proposal for a grant to publish the journals I kept in treatment years ago. More in a sense of collected documented useful info and artistic interpretation or snap shots through expressivity rather than reporting. This will be helpful for the current opiate crisis and lack of collected research for the treatment of Bipolar 2. Waste of time or?

  1. Found your site through Time to Change. Fellow healthcare professional here, and enjoying your insightful writing about mental health. Looking forward to delving into the archives. Great work!

  2. Good topic, a lot of media quote research without clearly understanding it and create headlines solely designed catch the eye. There is a definite movement in media (both mainstream and social ) towards binary too, where something is either this or that , good or bad. Mental illness and treatment thereof is much more nuanced. I also wish that more articles actually linked properly to the research they quote.

    1. Such good points. I definitely agree with you about the binary. Maybe the world leads a little CBT to deal with all-or-nothing cognitive distortions 😉

  3. This post is very useful. I have tried to read scientific studies but I don’t understand certain terms. I’m going to reblog this for future reference. Thank you for sharing.

    1. It’s definitely hard to fully grasp the implications of scientific studies without knowing the jargon, and even then it can be hard to weed through all the details to get at the points that really matter.

        1. What if there was a collective decision made to withhold the truth about our diagnoses to all the visiting research teams and doctors interviewing us for their grad whatevers for lack of faith in the system as the observation that the answers being sought were irrelevant and avoiding of what is only feared to speak up about? What if I’m the only survivor?

Leave a Reply

Discover more from Mental Health @ Home

Subscribe now to keep reading and get access to the full archive.

Continue reading